Hoppa till sidinnehåll

Characteristics and varieties of poor readers

Publicerad:2006-05-05

Det är relativt enkelt för en lärare och andra vuxna att avgöra om ett barn har läsproblem. Däremot förefaller det inte lika enkelt att avgöra vad som är problemet och vilka pedagogiska insatser som erfordras. Ulrika Wolff identifierar i sin avhandling Characteristics and varieties of poor readers olika profiler av elever med varierande läsförmåga, vilka även relateras till olika kulturella och socioekonomiska bakgrundsfaktorer.

Författare

Ulrika Wolff

Handledare

Professor Jan-Eric Gustafsson, Göteborgs universitet

Opponent

Professor Linda Siegel, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada

Disputerat vid

GU – Göteborgs universitet

Disputationsdag

2005-09-02

Titel (eng)

Characteristics and varieties of poor readers

Institution

Institutionen för Pedagogik och Didaktik

Characteristics and varieties of poor readers

This thesis is based on four empirical studies. The overall aim was to identify and examine different profiles of reading. In Study I, two latent profile analyses were conducted with the aim to identify subgroups, or profiles, of reading performance of 9-year old Swedish students. The latent profile analyses focussed on four aspects of reading performance: reading of continuous texts, reading of document texts (maps, charts etc.), word reading and reading speed. Eight performances profiles were obtained in the first study (N=5099), and were replicated with highly similar profiles in the second study (N=4184). The largest profiles, good and average readers, exhibited even reading performance across measures, implying that reading is a skill with high transfer and generality, whereas poor readers exhibited more heterogeneous performance patterns. The most stable profiles across studies were high performing students, poor comprehenders and dyslexic students. The phonological deficit hypothesis of dyslexia was assumed, which states that a phonological impairment is supposed to be the underlying cause of the manifest reading and writing problems. In Study II, a battery for group screening of dyslexia among adults was designed. The battery consisted of a self-report questionnaire, four tasks topping phonology and one task tapping orthography. Administration time was 40 minutes. All tasks discriminated highly between a group of adults with dyslexia (N=50) and a control group (N=67). Thus, it was possible to meet the challenge of implementing a nonvocal, phonologically based group screening of dyslexia problems. Study III attempted to investigate the often observed association between visual creativity and dyslexia, evidence for which is mainly anecdotal. Suggested causes of this observed association include different brain structures or functions, or compensation for deficiency in the area of reading and writing. In two studies, the prevalence of dyslexia among art university students as compared to non-art university students was examined. A total of 286 art students and 282 non-art students participated. The screening was based on word reading, a phonological choice test (in the second study) and a self-report questionnaire. In both studies the art students showed, as well as reported, significantly more dyslexia signs than the non-art students. In Study IV the incidence of phonological and surface dyslexia among Swedish dyslexic university students were examined. Participants were 40 university students with dyslexia, 40 academic-level matched students and 40 younger reading-level matched students. Two different methodologies were used. Firstly, a regression method was applied, where performance scores on a phonological choice task were plotted against performance scores on an orthographic choice task and vice versa. Confidence intervals (90%) were derived from the regression of the control groups separately and superimposed on corresponding plots for the dyslexic group. When the academic-level control group was used as reference group, a substantial number of both phonological and surface dyslexia was found. However, when the reading-level group was used as a reference the phonological dyslexia subgroup remained, but the surface dyslexia subgroup virtually disappeared. Secondly, a latent profile analysis was conducted on the dyslexia group based on five phonological and four orthographic measures. Seven profiles were obtained, of which none clearly exhibited poor performance on the orthographic tasks but not on the phonological tasks or vice versa. These results suggest a deviant development in phonological dyslexics and a delayed development in surface dyslexics. It thus supports the phonological deficit hypothesis. Educational consequences for varying reading problems are discussed and instructional intervention is suggested.

Forskningsbevakningen presenteras i samarbete med

forskningsinstitutet Ifous

Läs mer
Stockholm

Skolbibliotek

Välkommen till Skolportens konferens för dig som leder eller arbetar i Skolbibliotek! Delta på plats i Stockholm eller digitalt via webbkonferensen. Ta del av den senaste forskningen och utvecklas i din yrkesroll. Missa inte att boka till bästa pris redan idag!
Läs mer och boka
Åk F–Vux
6–7 maj 2025
Digital temaföreläsning

Folk- och världsmusik

Skolportens digitala föreläsning för dig som är musiklärare i grundskolan! Titta när det passar dig mellan 10 februari och 17 mars. Förutom handfasta förslag på hur du kan undervisa om folk- och världsmusik får du med dig diskussionsfrågor inför samarbetet med kollegor, samt mängder av material att använda direkt i undervisningen.
Läs mer och boka
Åk 4–9
10 feb – 17 mar 2025
Dela via: 

Relaterade artiklar

Relaterat innehåll

Senaste magasinen

Läs mer

Nyhetsbrev